Research in Pharmaceutical Sciences

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year
: 2020  |  Volume : 15  |  Issue : 3  |  Page : 241--248

Effects of HDAC inhibitors on spatial memory and memory extinction in SPS-induced PTSD rats


Ahmad Mohammadi-Farani1, Ali Pourmotabbed2, Yazdan Ardeshirizadeh3,  
1 Pharmaceutical Sciences Research Center, Health Institute; Department of Pharmacology, Toxicology and Medical Services, School of Pharmacy, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, I.R. Iran
2 Department of Physiology, School of Medicine, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, I.R. Iran
3 Student Research Committee, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, I.R. Iran

Correspondence Address:
Ahmad Mohammadi-Farani
Pharmaceutical Sciences Research Center, Health Institute; Department of Pharmacology, Toxicology and Medical Services, School of Pharmacy, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah
I.R. Iran

Abstract

Background and purpose: Neurobiological changes in memory processes seem to play a role in the pathophysiology of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Memory itself is influenced by PTSD, too. Histone deacetylase inhibitors (HDAIs) have shown promising results in the extinction of fear-related memories in animals and hence they seem to be important for the treatment of PTSD. Data are scarce about the effect of HDAIs in spatial memory formation/extinction in PTSD models. The main goal of the present work is to find the effect of sodium butyrate (NaBu), as an HDAI, on spatial memory and spatial memory extinction in rats exposed to single prolonged stress procedure (SPS). Experimental approach: Different doses of NaBu were administered subcutaneously for 7 days in different groups of rats after SPS procedure. Learning, memory, and extinction of memory were evaluated in the Morris water maze test of spatial memory in 6 consecutive days. Findings / Results: The results show that NaBu (0.5 mg/kg) alleviates impaired learning and memory in SPS rats. It also facilitates the extinction of newly formed memory in the animals. Conclusion and implications: Our data suggest that the administration of HDAIs after a traumatic experience can prevent the aversive effects of SPS on spatial memory. It also reinforces the notion that extinction of spatial memory involves the same or similar brain circuitry that is involved in the extinction of fear memories in PTSD patients.



How to cite this article:
Mohammadi-Farani A, Pourmotabbed A, Ardeshirizadeh Y. Effects of HDAC inhibitors on spatial memory and memory extinction in SPS-induced PTSD rats.Res Pharma Sci 2020;15:241-248


How to cite this URL:
Mohammadi-Farani A, Pourmotabbed A, Ardeshirizadeh Y. Effects of HDAC inhibitors on spatial memory and memory extinction in SPS-induced PTSD rats. Res Pharma Sci [serial online] 2020 [cited 2022 Dec 2 ];15:241-248
Available from: https://www.rpsjournal.net/text.asp?2020/15/3/241/288426


Full Text



 Introduction



Fear-related disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), often are persistent disorders which are not easy to treat. A lot of patients remain symptomatic after initial therapy [1]. The exact neurobiology of PTSD is not known but it is strongly believed that it is the result of altered neural connections in the brain which occurs after traumatic stress [2]. Understanding these neurobiological changes can help scientists develop new therapeutic strategies.

There is strong evidence that PTSD is correlated to some changes that occur in the processing of memory. One of these processes is called fear conditioning. Fear conditioning is established when a neutral or conditioned stimulus is paired with an aversive or unconditioned stimulus. This pairing changes the plasticity of the fear system and hence the fear response of the individual [3]. Another important PTSD-related process is called fear of extinction. Extinction occurs when individuals repeatedly receive conditioned stimuli without the unconditioned stimuli.

It seems that extinction is a new form of memory that alters the plasticity of multiple brain regions [4]. PTSD also has negative consequences on memory. Some studies show that animals exposed to single-prolonged stress (SPS), an animal model for PTSD, have impaired spatial memory [5],[6]. Other studies demonstrate that PTSD in humans is also followed by spatial memory deficits [7],[8],[9].

Histone deacetylase inhibitors (HDAIs) are agents that control the action of a group of enzymes called histone deacetylases. These enzymes reduce histone acetylation and by relaxing the chromatin structure they have negative effects on gene transcription. HDAIs promote gene transcription and protein synthesis which is a required step in the formation of both fear conditioning [10] and fear extinction memories [11].

A number of previous works have demonstrated the facilitating effect of HDAIs in contextual or cued fear extinction models [12],[15] but little is known about their effect on spatial memory function. This study aimed to investigate the effect of sodium butyrate (NaBu), as an HDAI, on spatial memory and spatial memory extinction in normal versus SPS-induced PTSD rats. In this study, we actually wanted to investigate the preventive potential of the drug on cognitive symptoms of PTSD. There are different approaches to PTSD prevention. Primary prevention treatment is started before the traumatic event; secondary prevention applies when the drug is administered between the traumatic event and development of PTSD, and tertiary prevention is a situation in which treatment is initiated when the first symptoms of PTSD become apparent [1]. In the setting of our experiments we gave the drug to the animals during a week after the SPS procedure (traumatic event), therefore this study indicated a secondary preventive approach.

 Materials and Methods



Animals

Male Wistar rats weighing 250-300 g were used in the experiments. Subjects were housed (3 per cage) in a temperature-controlled environment with a 12/12-h light/dark cycle.

Animal use was in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Medical University of Kermanshah I.R. Iran under the ethical code: 1395.358).

Animal grouping

Five groups of eight animals each were used in the study; [1] control group, which did not go through SPS procedure. This group received normal saline as vehicle and all tests are performed on them; [2] PTSD group 1 (vehicle), who went through SPS and receives vehicle; [3] PTSD group 2, who received NaBu at 0.1 mg/kg; [4] PTSD group 3, who received NaBu at 0.5 mg/kg; and [5] PTSD group 4, who received NaBu at 1 mg/kg.

Single-prolonged stress procedure

SPS was carried out according to the procedure described before [16],[17]. At first, Rats were restrained for 2 h in a clear polyethylene cone. Immediately after this stage they were put into a circular pool of water (60 cm diameter, 24 °C), where they were forced to swim for 20 min. Then they were recuperated for a 15-min period. As the final step rats were exposed to diethyl ether until they became consciousness and then they were transferred to their home cages and remained for 7 days.

Drug administration

Immediately after SPS and for the next seven consecutive days, rats received a subcutaneous injection of the drug or vehicle.

Morris water maze apparatus

As described previously [18] the maze is a circular water tank that has a diameter of 1.5 m and a height of 80 cm. It is filled with tap water to a height of 30 cm. The temperature of the water is set at 25 °C. There is a scape platform (diameter, 20 cm) which is placed 2 cm beneath the water surface. A black curtain surrounded the pool and some visual cues were pinned to it. A video camera was installed on top of the pool which recorded animal movements. The recorded films were computerized and analyzed by the tracking system EthoVision XT6 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands).

Behavioral procedure

The test was performed according to Méndez-Couz et al. with slight modifications [19]. Before the training sessions, rats had habituation sessions for 2 days. On the first day, the platform was fixed at the center of the empty maze. Rats were put on the platform for 60 s. On the second day, the tank was full of water and the rats were put on the platform for another 60 s. If rats jumped off the platform they were guided back to it.

Reference memory task

The training phase consisted of one session of four trials in 6 consecutive days. In each trial, rats were put in the water from the central border of each virtually determined quadrants of north-east, north-west, south-east, and southwest in a pseudorandom order. The platform was hidden under the water and the rats searched for it. The position of the platform was constant. The duration of each trial was 60 s. each rat had a 2 min inter-trial time during which it was towel-dried and kept in a heated cage. If rats could find the platform they were allowed to rest on it for 15 s and if they could not find the platform in the 60 s period they were gently guided to it and let to stay for 15 s [19].

Retention probe test

On the seventh day, a retention probe trial was carried out to test the spatial memory of the animals. In this test, the platform was removed from the pool and rats were released in water from the opposite quadrant of the platform. They were allowed to swim for 60 s [19].

Extinction tests

For the extinction test, we followed the protocol by Rossato et al. [20]. The day after retention probe test animals had four extinction sessions. Each session had four trials in which each rat were allowed to swim in the pool for 60 s. animals had a 60 s inter-trial resting time during the tests. The platform was removed from the maze in extinction trials [19].

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed by Sigmaplot 12 software (Systat Software, Chicago, USA). Differences in the distance to reach the platform in the training phase for each group were analyzed using one-way repeated-measures ANOVA. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to find the difference between the main effects of day and treatment in SPS and control groups. Data from the retention and extinction probe trials were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA. In cases of significant ANOVA results, Tukey’s post hoc tests were used to compare the mean differences. Results of the retention probe and the extinction probe tests were analyzed by the Student’s t-test when the SPS group was compared to the control. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM.

 Results



Effects of SPS on learning and memory in the Morris water maze test

As shown in [Figure 1]A both normal and SPS rats learned to find the platform during the 6 testing days (control, P = 0.002; PTSD, P < 0.001). Further analysis revealed a significant group (P = 0.01) and days (P < 0.001) effect, but no group and day interaction (P = 0.5). Post hoc analysis indicated a significant difference on the 4th, 5th and 6th days (P < 0.05).{Figure 1}

The results of comparing mean distance in target quadrant in normal and SPS conditions [Figure 1]B showed that there was a significant difference in the distance for control and SPS groups; P < 0.05. These results suggest that SPS decreases the memory for platform position in rats.

Effects of NaBu on learning and memory in PTSD rats

Mean distance to the platform was compared among drug-treated PTSD animals [Figure 2]A. Analysis of data showed that there is a significant effect for the main factors of day (P < 0.05) and dose of NaBu (P < 0.05). And there is no interaction for these main factors (P = 0.17). Further analysis showed that compared to the vehicle group the drug could decrease the distance to the platform at the dose of 0.5 mg/kg (P < 0.5).{Figure 2}

For the retention probe trial [Figure 2]B the results showed that the means of the groups are statistically different from each other (P = 0.017) and the drug at 0.5 mg/kg could increase the traveling distance of the animals in the target quadrant (P < 0.05) in comparison to the vehicle group.

Effects of SPS on memory extinction in the Morris water maze test

As regards the effects of SPS on memory extinction [Figure 3] the results showed that animals in PTSD group spent more time in the target quadrant in extinction sessions (P < 0.001).{Figure 3}

Effects of NaBu on memory extinction in SPS animals

As shown in [Figure 4] mean distance of animals in the target quadrant tend to have a U shape pattern. A significant difference was observed between the vehicle-treated group and the group that received the drug at 0.5 mg/kg (P < 0.05).{Figure 4}

Effects of NaBu on animal velocity in SPS groups

Earlier results for learning, memory, and extinction can be confounded by the speed of the animals in the tests. Therefore, the mean velocities through the retention probe test were compared in different groups. The results showed that the speed of animals in different groups could not be considered statistically different from each other (P = 0.1, [Figure 5]).{Figure 5}

 Discussion



In this study, we demonstrated that SPS not only impairs spatial learning and memory but also disrupts the process of spatial memory extinction in rats. We also showed that NaBu, as a HDAI, can reverse the deleterious effects of SPS on both consolidation and extinction of spatial memory when administered after the traumatic experience.

Memory dysfunction is considered as part of the diagnostic criteria for PTSD [21]. Many studies show that PTSD imposes impairment in declarative memory in humans by disrupting several aspects of hippocampal function [22],[23],[24],[25],[26]. Regarding the importance of the hippocampus in declarative memory in humans and its critical role in rodent spatial memory [27],[28] it is reasonable to propose that PTSD in rodents will lead to deficits in spatial memory. SPS is an accepted procedure that induces PTSD-like symptoms in rats [5],[6]. The present findings showed that both learning and memory [Figure 1]A and [Figure 1]B are affected by the SPS procedure. SPS rats had an overall increased time to find the platform in the testing days. They also spent more time in the platform quadrant on the probe trial. These results are consistent with other evidence that SPS leads to spatial memory deficits in the Morris water maze test [5],[6],[29]. Harvey et al. focused their studies on the possible effect of PTSD on the serotonergic system in the hippocampus of rats. They found that as an adaptive reaction to severe stress hippocampal 5HT1A receptor density increases. Since 5HT1A receptors are critical for the regulation and function of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis it was concluded that it can be an underlying cause for the hypersensitivity of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis and decreased levels of cortisol in PTSD animals. As part of their experiments they have assessed the spatial memory of rats exposed to SPS and reported a decline in related behavioral parameters [5]. In another study it was shown that one-week SPS rats showed enhanced acoustic startle response, the deficit in spatial memory in MWM, and elevated levels of fear in a fear conditioning model [6]. A more recent study also found that SPS procedure led to impairment of place learning task in a plus-maze apparatus. It also showed that SPS slowed the correct responses in the extinction training in the maze [29].

Regulation of gene expression is a required step in the formation of long term memory. Levenson et al. first reported the effects of HDAIs; trichostatin A and NaBu on memory formation in the hippocampus. in vitro experiments showed that these inhibitors enhanced long term potentiation in CA1 region of the hippocampus, a mechanism that underlies long term memory formation. Administration of NaBu before contextual fear conditioning also facilitated the formation of long-term memory in rats [30]. Other researchers showed that hyperacetylation of histone proteins at the promoter of genes that are important for memory formation like cAMP response element-binding protein and nuclear factor-κB is associated with enhanced memory for objects and space [31]. In concert with these studies, our results show that in SPS exposed animals NaBu (0.5 mg/kg) enhanced spatial learning of rats indicated by decreased distance to the platform on 3rd ,4th, and 5th days of training [Figure 2]A. As evident from increased traveling distance of animals in platform quadrant, memory is also improved in SPS exposed rats by the same dose of the drug [Figure 2]B. Both higher (1 mg/kg) and lower (0.1 mg/kg) doses of the drug have not a statistically significant difference to the PTSD group [Figure 2]. The mechanistic reasons behind this inconsistency cannot be answered by our experiments. As it was observed in many other pharmacologic dose-response experiments, the effect of NaBu also complies with an inverted U shape pattern in our study [18].

Pavlovian fear conditioning is a widely used model to study mechanisms of fear formation and extinction. In this model conditioned fear responses are extinguished when the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus [32]. It is proposed that some clinical symptoms of PTSD are developed due to dysfunction of the fear extinction processes in the brain [33],[34]. Most behavioral models of extinction measure the time before losing the memory of environmental insults as an index for fear extinction [35]. Some researchers propose that the extinction of the memory of the platform position in Morris water maze follows the basis of extinction in other operant conditioning models [19],[36],[37]. Our results showed [Figure 3] that the mean distance that SPS rats travel in the target quadrant in the extinction probe test is increased after 6 days of the training procedure. This indicates that they are less inclined to forget the position of the platform and hence it is similar to classical conditioning models in which memory of fear is harder to elude for SPS subjected rats [38],[39],[40],[41],[42].

Further results showed that NaBu (0.5 mg/kg) could decrease the mean distance of traveling of animals in the extinction test [Figure 4]. This means that the drug has helped to extinguish the memory of the platform position in the maze. These changes have occurred independent of the locomotive behaviors of the animals since the speed of movement is similar among different groups [Figure 5]. It is highly believed that agents that are capable of enhancing the mechanisms of extinction processes would be promising drugs to treat a variety of neurological disorders such as PTSD, specific phobias, drug abuse, etc. [43]. Stafford et al. showed that systemic, intra-hippocampal, and intra-medial prefrontal cortex administration of NaBu in a contextual fear conditioning model induced behavioral (i.e. reduction of freezing) and molecular (i.e. increased histone acetylation and c-Fos expression in the infralimbic cortex) changes that are indicative of a strong extinction process in the brain [12]. Other studies also point out the efficacy of HDAIs as extinction enhancers [13],[14],[15]. These studies have used fear-based conditioning methodologies. Spatial learning is more like a hippocampal-dependent learning process while in conditioning models other structures such as the amygdala play important roles [19]. Therefore, it seems that regardless of the brain regions involved in the learning process, extinction learning depends on a structure that is involved in both types of learning. This site is affected by the traumatic SPS procedure and it may be the site of action for HDAIs.

 Conclusion



Our research showed that NaBu as an HDAI can reverse the noxious effects of SPS on spatial memory when administered on the consolidation phase after the SPS procedure. It also facilitated the process of extinction in the Morris water maze. Altogether, these results proposed that HDAIs are good candidates both for preventive treatment of PTSD or to cure learning impairments in the aftermath of a traumatic event. Further studies are necessary to fully understand the site and mechanism of action of their effect.

 Acknowledgements



The content of this paper is extracted from the Pharm. D thesis submitted by Yazdan Ardeshirzadeh which was financially supported (Grant No. 95324) by the Research Council of Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, I.R. Iran.

 Conflict of Interest Statement



All authors declare no conflict of interest for this study.

 Authors’ Contribution



A. Mohammadi-Farani and A. Pourmotabbed conceived of the main concepts and planed the experiments. A. Mohammadi- Farani and Y. Ardeshirizadeh implemented literature search. All authors contributed to the final manuscript. A. Pourmotabbed verified the analytical methods. Y. Ardeshirizadeh performed the experiments and acquired data under supervision of A. Mohammadi-Farani.

References

1Miao XR, Chen QB, Wei K, Tao KM, Lu ZJ. Posttraumatic stress disorder: from diagnosis to prevention. Mil Med Res. 2018;5(1):32-38. DOI: 10.1186/s40779-018-0179-0.
2Abdallah CG, Southwick SM, Krystal JH. Neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a path from novel pathophysiology to innovative therapeutics. Neurosci Lett. 2017;649:130-132. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.04.046.
3Cain CK, Maynard GD, Kehne JH. Targeting memory processes with drugs to prevent or cure PTSD. Expert Opin Investig Drugs. 2012;21(9):1323-1350. DOI: 10.1517/13543784.2012.704020.
4Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, Maren S. Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):352-360. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015.
5Harvey BH, Naciti C, Brand L, Stein DJ. Endocrine, cognitive and hippocampal/cortical 5HT 1A/2A receptor changes evoked by a time-dependent sensitisation (TDS) stress model in rats. Brain Res. 2003;983(1-2):97-107. DOI: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)03033-6.
6Kohda K, Harada K, Kato K, Hoshino A, Motohashi J, Yamaji T, et al. Glucocorticoid receptor activation is involved in producing abnormal phenotypes of single-prolonged stress rats: a putative post-traumatic stress disorder model. Neuroscience. 2007;148(1):22- 33. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.05.041.
7Tempesta D, Mazza M, Iaria G, De Gennaro L, Ferrara M. A specific deficit in spatial memory acquisition in post-traumatic stress disorder and the role of sleep in its consolidation. Hippocampus. 2012;22(5):1154-1163. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20961.
8Smith KV, Burgess N, Brewin CR, King JA. Impaired allocentric spatial processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;119:69-76. DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.007.
9Miller JK, McDougall S, Thomas S, Wiener JM. Impairment in active navigation from trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017;140:114-123. DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.019.
10Monsey MS, Ota KT, Akingbade IF, Hong ES, Schafe GE. Epigenetic alterations are critical for fear memory consolidation and synaptic plasticity in the lateral amygdala. PLoS One. 2011;6(5):e19958,1-13. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019958.
11Whittle N, Singewald N. HDAC inhibitors as cognitive enhancers in fear, anxiety and trauma therapy: where do we stand? Biochem Soc Trans. 2014;42(2):569-581. DOI: 10.1042/BST20130233.
12Stafford JM, Raybuck JD, Ryabinin AE, Lattal KM. Increasing histone acetylation in the hippocampus- infralimbic network enhances fear extinction. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72(1):25-33. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.012.
13Whittle N, Schmuckermair C, Gunduz Cinar O, Hauschild M, Ferraguti F, Holmes A, et al. Deep brain stimulation, histone deacetylase inhibitors and glutamatergic drugs rescue resistance to fear extinction in a genetic mouse model. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:414-423. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.06.001.
14Lattal KM, Barrett RM, Wood MA. Systemic or intrahippocampal delivery of histone deacetylase inhibitors facilitates fear extinction. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121(5):1125-1131. DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1125.
15Itzhak Y, Anderson KL, Kelley JB, Petkov M. Histone acetylation rescues contextual fear conditioning in nNOS KO mice and accelerates extinction of cued fear conditioning in wild type mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2012;97(4):409-417. DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.03.005.
16Liberzon I, Krstov M, Young EA. Stress-restress: effects on ACTH and fast feedback. Psychoneuroendocrino. 1997;22(6):443-453. DOI: 10.1016/s0306-4530(97)00044-9.
17Mohammad Alizadeh MA, Abrari K, Lashkar Blouki T, Ghorbanian MT, Jadidi M. Pulsed electromagnetic field attenuated PTSD-induced failure of conditioned fear extinction. Iran J Basic Med Sci. 2019;22(6):650- 659. DOI: 10.22038/ijbms.2019.32576.7797.
18Mohammadi-Farani A, Haghighi A, Ghazvineh M. Effects of long term administration of testosterone and estradiol on spatial memory in rats. Res Pharm Sci. 2015;10(5):407-418.
19Mendez-Couz M, Conejo NM, Vallejo G, Arias JL. Spatial memory extinction: a c-Fos protein mapping study. Behav Brain Res. 2014;260:101-110. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.032.
20Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LR, Medina JH, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Retrieval induces hippocampal- dependent reconsolidation of spatial memory. Learn Mem. 2006;13(4):431-440. DOI: 10.1101/lm.315206.
21Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Fifth edition. DOI: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.
22Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Nazeer A, et al. MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(5):924-932. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp. 160.5.924.
23Elzinga BM, Bremner JD. Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? J Affect Disord. 2002;70(1):1-17. DOI: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00351-2.
24Fenster RJ, Lebois LAM, Ressler KJ, Suh J. Brain circuit dysfunction in post-traumatic stress disorder: from mouse to man. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(9):535-551. DOI: 10.1038/s41583-018-0039-7.
25Bremner JD, Scott TM, Delaney RC, Southwick SM, Mason JW, Johnson DR, et al. Deficits in short-term memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(7):1015-1019. DOI: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1015.
26Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Staib LH, et al. Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(10):879-889. DOI: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01891-7.
27Diamond DM, Fleshner M, Ingersoll N, Rose GM. Psychological stress impairs spatial working memory: relevance to electrophysiological studies of hippocampal function. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110(4):661-672. DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.4.661.
28Luine V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS. Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res. 1994;639(1):167-170. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7.
29Goodman J, McIntyre CK. Impaired spatial memory and enhanced habit memory in a rat model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Front Pharmacol. 2017;8:663-670. DOI: 10.3389/fphar.2017.00663.
30Levenson JM, O’Riordan KJ, Brown KD, Trinh MA, Molfese DL, Sweatt JD. Regulation of histone acetylation during memory formation in the hippocampus. J Biol Chem. 2004;279(39):40545- 40559. DOI: 10.1074/jbc.M402229200.
31Koshibu K, Graff J, Beullens M, Heitz FD, Berchtold D, Russig H, et al. Protein phosphatase 1 regulates the histone code for long-term memory. J Neurosci. 2009;29(41):13079-13089. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3610-09.2009.
32Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biol Psychol. 2006;73(1):61-71. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008.
33Quirk GJ, Garcia R, Gonzalez-Lima F. Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):337-343. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.010.
34Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research-past, present, and future. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):376- 382. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.004.
35Singewald N, Holmes A. Rodent models of impaired fear extinction. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2019;236(1):21-32. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-018-5054-x.
36Prados J, Sansa J, Artigas AA. Partial reinforcement effects on learning and extinction of place preferences in the water maze. Learn Behav. 2008;36(4):311-318. DOI: 10.3758/LB.36.4.311.
37Huston JP, Schulz D, Topic B. Toward an animal model of extinction-induced despair: focus on aging and physiological indices. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2009;116(8):1029-1036. DOI: 10.1007/s00702-009-0210-4.
38Knox D, George SA, Fitzpatrick CJ, Rabinak CA, Maren S, Liberzon I. Single prolonged stress disrupts retention of extinguished fear in rats. Learn Mem. 2012;19(2):43-49. DOI: 10.1101/lm.024356.111.
39Ledgerwood L, Richardson R, Cranney J. D- cycloserine and the facilitation of extinction of conditioned fear: consequences for reinstatement. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118(3):505-513. DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.505.
40Lin CC, Tung CS, Lin PH, Huang CL, Liu YP. Traumatic stress causes distinctive effects on fear circuit catecholamines and the fear extinction profile in a rodent model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(9):1484-1495. DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.06.004.
41Souza RR, Robertson NM, Pruitt DT, Gonzales PA, Hays SA, Rennaker RL, et al. Vagus nerve stimulation reverses the extinction impairments in a model of PTSD with prolonged and repeated trauma. Stress. 2019;22(4):509-520. DOI: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1602604.
42Yamamoto S, Morinobu S, Iwamoto Y, Ueda Y, Takei S, Fujita Y, et al. Alterations in the hippocampal glycinergic system in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(15):1069-1074. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.013.
43Smith NB, Doran JM, Sippel LM, Harpaz-Rotem I. Fear extinction and memory reconsolidation as critical components in behavioral treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder and potential augmentation of these processes. Neurosci Lett. 2017;649:170-175. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.01.006.